home *** CD-ROM | disk | FTP | other *** search
- $Unique_ID{how01077}
- $Pretitle{}
- $Title{Descent Of Man, The
- Chapter 8.2}
- $Subtitle{}
- $Author{Darwin, Charles}
- $Affiliation{}
- $Subject{male
- males
- sexes
- female
- sex
- characters
- females
- animals
- selection
- alone}
- $Date{1874}
- $Log{}
- Title: Descent Of Man, The
- Book: Part II: Sexual Selection
- Author: Darwin, Charles
- Date: 1874
-
- Chapter 8.2
-
- The Male Generally More Modified than the Female. - Throughout the animal
- kingdom when the sexes differ in external appearance, it is, with rare
- exceptions, the male which has been the more modified; for, generally, the
- female retains a closer resemblance to the young of her own species and to
- other adult members of the same group. The cause of this seems to lie in the
- males of almost all animals having stronger passions than the females. Hence
- it is the males that fight together and sedulously display their charms before
- the females; and the victors transmit their superiority to their male
- offspring. Why both sexes do not thus acquire the characters of their fathers
- will be considered hereafter. That the males of all mammals eagerly pursue
- the females is notorious to every one. So it is with birds; but many cock
- birds do not so much pursue the hen, as display their plumage, perform strange
- antics, and pour forth their song in her presence. The male in the few fish
- observed seems much more eager than the female; and the same is true of
- alligators, and apparently of Batrachians. Throughout the enormous class of
- insects, as Kirby remarks, ^450 "the law is that the male shall seek the
- female." Two good authorities, Mr. Blackwall and Mr. D. C. Spence Bate, tell
- me that the males of spiders and crustaceans are more active and more erratic
- in their habits than the females. When the organs of sense or locomotion are
- present in the one sex of insects and crustaceans and absent in the other, or
- when, as is frequently the case, they are more highly developed in the one
- than in the other, it is, as far as I can discover, almost invariably the male
- which retains such organs, or has them most developed; and this shows that the
- male is the more active member in the courtship of the sexes. ^451
-
- [Footnote 450: Kirby and Spence, "Introduction to Entomology," vol. iii, 1826,
- p. 342.]
-
- [Footnote 451: One parasitic Hymenopterous insect (Westwood, "Modern Class. of
- Insects," vol. ii, p. 160) forms an exception to the rule, as the male has
- rudimentary wings, and never quits the cell in which it is born, while the
- female has well-developed wings. Audouin believes that the females of this
- species are impregnated by the males which are born in the same cells with
- them; but it is much more probable that the females visit other cells, so that
- close interbreeding is thus avoided. We shall hereafter meet in various
- classes, with a few exceptional cases, in which the female, instead of the
- male, is the seeker and wooer.]
-
- The female, on the other hand, with the rarest exceptions, is less eager
- than the male. As the illustrious Hunter ^452 long ago observed she generally
- "requires to be courted;" she is coy, and may often be seen endeavoring for a
- long time to escape from the male. Every observer of the habits of animals
- will be able to call to mind instances of this kind. It is shown by various
- facts, given hereafter, and by the results fairly attributable to sexual
- selection, that the female, though comparatively passive, generally exerts
- some choice and accepts one male in preference to others. Or she may accept,
- as appearances would sometimes lead us to believe, not the male which is the
- most attractive to her, but the one which is the least distasteful. The
- exertion of some choice on the part of the female seems a law almost as
- general as the eagerness of the male.
-
- [Footnote 452: "Essays and Observations," edited by Owen, vol. i, 1861, p.
- 194.]
-
- We are naturally led to inquire why the male, in so many and such
- distinct classes, has become more eager than the female, so that he searches
- for her and plays the more active part in courtship. It would be no advantage
- and some loss of power if each sex searched for the other; but why should the
- male almost always be the seeker? The ovules of plants after fertilization
- have to be nourished for a time; hence the pollen is necessarily brought to
- the female organs - being placed on the stigma, by means of insects or the
- wind, or by the spontaneous movements of the stamens; and in the Algae, etc.,
- by the locomotive power of the antherozooids. With lowly-organized aquatic
- animals, permanently affixed to the same spot and having their sexes separate,
- the male element is invariably brought to the female; and of this we can see
- the reason, for even if the ova were detached before fertilization, and did
- not require subsequent nourishment or protection, there would yet be greater
- difficulty in transporting them than the male element, because, being larger
- than the latter, they are produced in far smaller numbers. So that many of
- the lower animals are, in this respect, analogous with plants. ^453 The males
- of affixed and aquatic animals having been led to emit their fertilizing
- elements in this way, it is natural that any of their descendants, which rose
- in the scale and became locomotive, should retain the same habit; and they
- would approach the female as closely as possible, in order not to risk the
- loss of the fertilizing element in a long passage of it through the water.
- With some few of the lower animals, the females alone are fixed, and the males
- of these must be the seekers. But it is difficult to understand why the males
- of species, of which the progenitors were primordially free, should invariably
- have acquired the habit of approaching the females, instead of being
- approached by them. But in all cases, in order that the males should seek
- efficiently, it would be necessary that they should be endowed with strong
- passions; and the acquirement of such passions would naturally follow from the
- more eager leaving a larger number of offspring than the less eager.
-
- [Footnote 453: Prof. Sachs ("Lehrbuch der Botanik," 1870, s. 633), in speaking
- of the male and female reproductive cells, remarks, "verhalt sich die eine bei
- der Vereinigung activ, . . . die andere erscheint bei der Vereinigung
- passiv."]
-
- The great eagerness of the males has thus indirectly led to their much
- more frequently developing secondary sexual characters than the females. But
- the development of such characters would be much aided if the males were more
- liable to vary than the females - as I concluded they were - after a long
- study of domesticated animals. Von Nathusius, who has had very wide
- experience, is strongly of the same opinion. ^454 Good evidence also in favor
- of this conclusion can be produced by a comparison of the two sexes in
- mankind. During the Novara Expedition ^455 a vast number of measurements was
- made of various parts of the body in different races, and the men were found
- in almost every case to present a greater range of variation than the women;
- but I shall have to recur to this subject in a future chapter. Mr. J. Wood,
- ^456 who has carefully attended to the variation of the muscles in man, puts
- in italics the conclusion that "the greatest number of abnormalities in each
- subject is found in the males." He had previously remarked that "altogether in
- 102 subjects, the varieties of redundancy were found to be half as many again
- as in females, contrasting widely with the greater frequency of deficiency in
- females before described." Prof. Macalister likewise remarks ^457 that
- variations in the muscles "are probably more common in males than females."
- Certain muscles which are not normally present in mankind are also more
- frequently developed in the male than in the female sex, although exceptions
- to this rule are said to occur. Dr. Burt Wilder ^458 has tabulated the cases
- of 152 individuals with supernumerary digits, of which 86 were males and 39,
- or less than half, females, the remaining 27 being of unknown sex. It should
- not, however, be overlooked that women would more frequently endeavor to
- conceal a deformity of this kind than men. Again, Dr. L. Meyer asserts that
- the ears of man are more variable in form than those of a woman. ^459 Lastly,
- the temperature is more variable in man than in woman. ^460
-
- [Footnote 454: "Vortrage uber Viehzucht," 1872, p. 63.]
-
- [Footnote 455: "Reise der Novara; Anthropolog. Theil," 1867, ss. 216-269. The
- results were calculated by Dr. Weisbach from measurements made by Drs. K.
- Scherzer and Schwarz. On the greater variability of the males of domesticated
- animals, see my "Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication," vol.
- ii, 1868, p. 75.]
-
- [Footnote 456: "Proceedings Royal Soc.," vol. xvi, July, 1868, pp. 519, 524.]
-
- [Footnote 457: "Proc. Royal Irish Academy," vol. x, 1868, p. 123.]
-
- [Footnote 458: "Massachusetts Medical Soc.," vol. ii, No. 3, 1868, p. 9.]
-
- [Footnote 459: "Archiv fur Path. Anat. und Phys.," 1871, p. 488.]
-
- [Footnote 460: The conclusions recently arrived at by Dr. J. Stockton Hough,
- on the temperature of man, are given in the "Pop. Science Review," Jan. 1,
- 1874, p. 97.]
-
- The cause of the greater general variability in the male sex than in the
- female is unknown, except in so far as secondary sexual characters are
- extraordinarily variable and are usually confined to the males; and, as we
- shall presently see, this fact is, to a certain extent, intelligible. Through
- the action of sexual and natural selection male animals have been rendered in
- very many instances widely different from their females; but independently of
- selection the two sexes, from differing constitutionally, tend to vary in a
- somewhat different manner. The female has to expend much organic matter in the
- formation of her ova, whereas the male expends much force in fierce contests
- with his rivals, in wandering about in search of the female, in exerting his
- voice, pouring out odoriferous secretions, etc.; and this expenditure is
- generally concentrated within a short period. The great vigor of the male
- during the season of love seems often to intensify his colors independently of
- any marked difference from the female. ^461 In mankind, and even as low down
- in the organic scale as in the Lepidoptera, the temperature of the body is
- higher in the male than in the female, accompanied in the case of man by a
- slower pulse. ^462 On the whole, the expenditure of matter and force by the
- two sexes is probably nearly equal, though effected in very different ways and
- at different rates.
-
- [Footnote 461: Prof. Mantegazza is inclined to believe ("Lettera a Carlo
- Darwin," "Archivio per l'Anthropologia," 1871, p. 306) that the bright colors,
- common in so many male animals, are due to the presence and retention by them
- of the spermatic fluid; but this can hardly be the case; for many male birds,
- for instance young pheasants, become brightly colored in the autumn of their
- first year.]
-
- [Footnote 462: For mankind, see Dr. J. Stockton Hough, whose conclusions are
- given in the "Pop. Science Review," 1874, p. 97. See Girard's observations on
- the Lepidoptera, as given in the "Zoological Record," 1869, p. 347.]
-
- From the causes just specified the two sexes can hardly fail to differ
- somewhat in constitution, at least during the breeding season; and although
- they may be subjected to exactly the same conditions they will tend to vary in
- a different manner. If such variations are of no service to either sex they
- will not be accumulated and increased by sexual or natural selection.
- Nevertheless, they may become permanent if the exciting cause acts
- permanently; and in accordance with a frequent form of inheritance they may be
- transmitted to that sex alone in which they first appeared. In this case the
- two sexes will come to present permanent, yet unimportant, differences of
- character. For instance, Mr. Allen shows that with a large number of birds
- inhabiting the northern and southern United States, the specimens from the
- south are darker-colored than those from the north; and this seems to be the
- direct result of the difference in temperature, light, etc., between the two
- regions. Now, in some few cases, the two sexes of the same species appear to
- have been differently affected; in the Ageloeus phoeniceus the males have had
- their colors greatly intensified in the south; whereas with Cardinalis
- virginianus it is the females which have been thus affected; with Quiscalus
- major the females have been rendered extremely variable in tint, while the
- males remain nearly uniform. ^463
-
- [Footnote 463: "Mammals and Birds of E. Florida," pp. 234, 280, 295.]
-
- A few exceptional cases occur in various classes of animals, in which the
- females instead of the males have acquired well-pronounced secondary sexual
- characters, such as brighter colors, greater size, strength or pugnacity.
- With birds there has sometimes been a complete transposition of the ordinary
- characters proper to each sex; the females having become the more eager in
- courtship, the males remaining comparatively passive, but apparently selecting
- the more attractive females, as we may infer from the results. Certain hen
- birds have thus been rendered more highly colored or otherwise ornamented, as
- well as more powerful and pugnacious than the cocks; these characters being
- transmitted to the female offspring alone.
-
- It may be suggested that in some cases a double process of selection has
- been carried on; that the males have selected the more attractive females and
- the latter the more attractive males. This, process, however, though it might
- lead to the modification of both sexes, would not make the one sex different
- from the other, unless indeed their tastes for the beautiful differed; but
- this is a supposition too improbable to be worth considering in the case of
- any animal, excepting man. There are, however, many animals in which the
- sexes resemble each other, both being furnished with the same ornaments, which
- analogy would lead us to attribute to the agency of sexual selection. In such
- cases it may be suggested with more plausibility that there has been a double
- or mutual process of sexual selection; the more vigorous and precocious
- females selecting the more attractive and vigorous males, the latter rejecting
- all except the more attractive females. But from what we know of the habits
- of animals, this view is hardly probable, for the male is generally eager to
- pair with any female. It is more probable that the ornaments common to both
- sexes were acquired by one sex, generally the male, and then transmitted to
- the offspring of both sexes. If, indeed, during a lengthened period the males
- of any species were greatly to exceed the females in number, and then during
- another lengthened period, but under different conditions, the reverse were to
- occur, a double, but not simultaneous, process of sexual selection might
- easily be carried on, by which the two sexes might be rendered widely
- different.
-
- We shall hereafter see that many animals exist, of which neither sex is
- brilliantly colored or provided with special ornaments, and yet the members of
- both sexes or of one alone have probably acquired simple colors, such as white
- or black, through sexual selection. The absence of bright tints or other
- ornaments may be the result of variations of the right kind never having
- occurred, or of the animals themselves having preferred plain black or white.
- Obscure tints have often been developed through natural selection for the sake
- of protection, and the acquirement through sexual selection of conspicuous
- colors appears to have been sometimes checked from the danger thus incurred.
- But in other cases the males during long ages may have struggled together for
- the possession of the females, and yet no effect will have been produced,
- unless a larger number of offspring were left by the more successful males to
- inherit their superiority than by the less successful; and this, as previously
- shown, depends on many complex contingencies.
-
- Sexual selection acts in a less rigorous manner than natural selection.
- The latter produces its effects by the life or death at all ages of the more
- or less successful individuals. Death, indeed, not rarely ensues from the
- conflicts of rival males. But generally the less successful male merely fails
- to obtain a female, or obtains a retarded and less vigorous female later in
- the season, or, if polygamous, obtains fewer females; so that they leave
- fewer, less vigorous, or no offspring. In regard to structures acquired
- through ordinary or natural selection there is in most cases, as long as the
- conditions of life remain the same, a limit to the amount of advantageous
- modification in relation to certain special purposes; but in regard to
- structures adapted to make one male victorious over another, either in
- fighting or in charming the female, there is no definite limit to the amount
- of advantageous modification; so that as long as the proper variations arise
- the work of sexual selection will go on. This circumstance may partly account
- for the frequent and extraordinary amount of variability presented by
- secondary sexual characters. Nevertheless, natural selection will determine
- that such characters shall not be acquired by the victorious males, if they
- would be highly injurious, either by expending too much of their vital powers
- or by exposing them to any great danger. The development, however, of certain
- structures - of the horns, for instance, in certain stags - has been carried
- to a wonderful extreme; and in some cases to an extreme which, as far as the
- general conditions of life are concerned, must be slightly injurious to the
- male. From this fact we learn that the advantages which favored males derive
- from conquering other males in battle or courtship, and thus leaving a
- numerous progeny, are in the long run greater than those derived from rather
- more perfect adaptation to their conditions of life. We shall further see,
- and it could never have been anticipated, that the power to charm the female
- has sometimes been more important than the power to conquer other males in
- battle.
-
- Laws of Inheritance. - In order to understand how sexual selection has
- acted on many animals of many classes, and in the course of ages has produced
- a conspicuous result, it is necessary to bear in mind the laws of inheritance
- as far as they are known. Two distinct elements are included under the term
- "inheritance" - the transmission and the development of characters; but as
- these generally go together the distinction is often overlooked. We see this
- distinction in those characters which are transmitted through the early years
- of life, but are developed only at maturity or during old age. We see the
- same distinction more clearly with secondary sexual characters, for these are
- transmitted through both sexes, though developed in one alone. That they are
- present in both sexes is manifest when two species having strongly marked
- sexual characters are crossed, for each transmits the characters proper to its
- own male and female sex to the hybrid offspring of either sex. The same fact
- is likewise manifest when characters proper to the male are occasionally
- developed in the female when she grows old or becomes diseased, as, for
- instance, when the common hen assumes the flowing tail-feathers, hackles,
- comb, spurs, voice, and even pugnacity of the cock. Conversely the same thing
- is evidence evident more or less plainly with castrated males. Again,
- independently of old age or disease, characters are occasionally transferred
- from the male to the female, as when in certain breeds of the fowl spurs
- regularly appear in the young and healthy females. But in truth they are
- simply developed in the female; for in every breed each detail in the
- structure of the spur is transmitted through the female to her male offspring.
- Many cases will hereafter be given where the female exhibits more or less
- perfectly characters proper to the male, in whom they must have been first
- developed and then transferred to the female. The converse case of the first
- development of characters in the female and of transference to the male is
- less frequent; it will therefore be well to give one striking instance. With
- bees the pollen-collecting apparatus is used by the female alone for gathering
- pollen for the larvae, yet in most of the species it is partially developed in
- the males to whom it is quite useless, and it is perfectly developed in the
- males of Bombus or the humble-bee. ^464 As not a single other Hymenopterous
- insect, not even the wasp, which is closely allied to the bee, is provided
- with a pollen-collecting apparatus, we have no grounds for supposing that male
- bees primordially suckled their young as well as the females; although we have
- some reason to suspect that male mammals primordially suckled their young as
- well as the females. Lastly, in all cases of reversion characters are
- transmitted through two, three or many more generations, and are then
- developed under certain unknown favorable conditions. This important
- distinction between transmission and development will be best kept in mind by
- the aid of the hypothesis of pangenesis. According to this hypothesis every
- unit or cell of the body throws off gemmules or undeveloped atoms, which are
- transmitted to the offspring of both sexes, and are multiplied by
- self-division. They may remain undeveloped during the early years of life or
- during successive generations; and their development into units or cells, like
- those from which they were derived, depends on their affinity for and union
- with other units or cells previously developed in the due order of growth.
-
- [Footnote 464: H. Muller, "Anwendung der Darwin'schen Lehre," etc. Verh. d.
- n. V. Jahrg. xxix, p. 42.]
-
- Inheritance at Corresponding Periods of Life. - This tendency is
- well-established. A new character, appearing in a young animal, whether it
- lasts throughout life or is only transient, will, in general, reappear in the
- offspring at the same age and last for the same time. If, on the other hand,
- a new character appears at maturity, or even during old age, it tends to
- reappear in the offspring at the same advanced age. When deviations from this
- rule occur, the transmitted characters much oftener appear before than after
- the corresponding age. As I have dwelt on this subject sufficiently in
- another work, ^465 I will here merely give two or three instances, for the
- sake of recalling the subject to the reader's mind. In several breeds of the
- fowl, the down-covered chickens, the young birds in their first true plumage,
- and the adults differ greatly from one another, as well as from their common
- parent-form, the Gallus bankiva; and these characters are faithfully
- transmitted by each breed to their offspring at the corresponding periods of
- life. For instance, the chickens of spangled Hamburgs, while covered with
- down, have a few dark spots on the head and rump, but are not striped
- longitudinally, as in many other breeds; in their first true plumage, "they
- are beautifully penciled," that is, each feather is transversely marked by
- numerous dark bars; but in their second plumage the feathers all become
- spangled or tipped with a dark round spot. ^466 Hence in this breed variations
- have occurred at, and been transmitted to, three distinct periods of life. The
- pigeon offers a more remarkable case, because the aboriginal parent-species
- does not undergo any change of plumage with advancing age, excepting at
- maturity the breast becomes more iridescent; yet there are breeds which do not
- acquire their characteristic colors until they have moulted two, three, or
- four times; and these modifications of plumage are regularly transmitted.
-
- [Footnote 465: "The Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication," vol.
- ii, 1868, p. 75. In the last chapter but one the provisional hypothesis of
- pangenesis, above alluded to, is fully explained.]
-
- [Footnote 466: These facts are given on the high authority of a great breeder,
- Mr. Teebay; see Tegetmeier's "Poultry Book," 1868, p. 158. On the characters
- of chickens of different breeds, and on the breeds of the pigeon, alluded to
- in the following paragraph, see "Variation of Animals," etc., vol. i, pp. 160,
- 249; vol. ii, p. 77.]
-
- Inheritance at Corresponding Seasons of the Year. - With animals in a
- state of nature, innumerable instances occur of characters appearing
- periodically at different seasons. We see this in the horns of the stag, and
- in the fur of the Arctic animals, which becomes thick and white during the
- winter. Many birds acquire bright colors and other decorations during the
- breeding-season alone. Pallas states, ^467 that in Siberia domestic cattle
- and horses become lighter-colored during the winter; and I have myself
- observed, and heard of similar strongly-marked changes of color, that is, from
- brownish-cream color or reddish-brown to a perfect white, in several ponies in
- England. Although I do not know that this tendency to change the color of the
- coat during different seasons is transmitted, yet it probably is so, as all
- shades of color are strongly inherited by the horse. Nor is this form of
- inheritance as limited by the seasons, more remarkable than its limitation by
- age or sex.
-
- [Footnote 467: "Novae species Quadrupedum e Glirium ordine," 1778, p. 7. On
- the transmission of color by the horse, see "Variation of Animals, etc., under
- Domestication," vol. i, p. 51. Also vol. ii, p. 71, for a general discussion
- on "Inheritance as Limited by Sex."]
-
- Inheritance as Limited by Sex. - The equal transmission of characters to
- both sexes is the commonest form of inheritance, at least with those animals
- which do not present strongly-marked sexual differences, and indeed with many
- of these. But characters are somewhat commonly transferred exclusively to
- that sex in which they first appear. Ample evidence on this head has been
- advanced in my work on "Variation Under Domestication," but a few instances
- may here be given. There are breeds of the sheep and goat, in which the horns
- of the male differ greatly in shape from those of the female; and these
- differences acquired under domestication are regularly transmitted to the same
- sex. As a rule, it is the females alone in cats which are tortoise-shell, the
- corresponding color in the males being rusty-red. With most breeds of the
- fowl the characters proper to each sex are transmitted to the same sex alone.
- So general is this form of transmission that it is an anomaly when variations
- in certain breeds are transmitted equally to both sexes. There are also
- certain sub-breeds of the fowl in which the males can hardly be distinguished
- from one another, while the females differ considerably in color. The sexes
- of the pigeon in the parent-species do not differ in any external character;
- nevertheless, in certain domesticated breeds the male is colored differently
- from the female. ^468 The wattle in the English carrier pigeon and the crop in
- the Pouter are more highly developed in the male than in the female; and
- although these characters have been gained through long-continued selection by
- man, the slight differences between the sexes are wholly due to the form of
- inheritance which has prevailed; for they have arisen, not from, but rather in
- opposition to, the wish of the breeder.
-
- [Footnote 468: Dr. Chapuis, "Le Pigeon Voyageur Belge," 1865, p. 87. Boitard
- et Corbie, "Les Pigeons de Voliere," etc., 1824, p. 173. See, also, on
- similar differences in certain breeds at Modena, "Le variazioni dei Colombi
- domestici," del Paolo Bonizzi, 1873.]
-
- Most of our domestic races have been formed by the accumulation of many
- slight variations; and as some of the successive steps have been transmitted
- to one sex alone, and some to both sexes, we find in the different breeds of
- the same species all gradations between great sexual dissimilarity and
- complete similarity. Instances have already been given with the breeds of the
- fowl and pigeon, and under nature analogous cases are common. With animals
- under domestication, but whether in nature I will not venture to say, one sex
- may lose characters proper to it, and may thus come somewhat to resemble the
- opposite sex; for instance, the males of some breeds of the fowl have lost
- their masculine tail-plumes and hackles. On the other hand, the differences
- between the sexes may be increased under domestication, as with merino sheep,
- in which the ewes have lost their horns. Again, characters proper to one sex
- may suddenly appear in the other sex; as in those sub-breeds of the fowl in
- which the hens acquire spurs while young; or, as in certain Polish sub-breeds,
- in which the females, as there is reason to believe, originally acquired a
- crest, and subsequently transferred it to the males. All these cases are
- intelligible on the hypothesis of pangenesis; for they depend on the gemmules
- of certain parts, although present in both sexes, becoming, through the
- influence of domestication, either dormant or developed in either sex.
-
- There is one difficult question which it will be convenient to defer to a
- future chapter; namely, whether a character at first developed in both sexes
- could through selection be limited in its development to one sex alone. If,
- for instance, a breeder observed that some of his pigeons (of which the
- characters are usually transferred in an equal degree to both sexes) varied
- into pale blue, could he by long-continued selection make a breed, in which
- the males alone should be of this tint, while the females remained unchanged?
- I will here only say that this, though perhaps not impossible, would be
- extremely difficult; for the natural result of breeding from the pale-blue
- males would be to change the whole stock of both sexes to this tint. If,
- however, variations of the desired tint appeared, which were from the first
- limited in their development to the male sex, there would not be the least
- difficulty in making a breed with the two sexes of a different color, as
- indeed has been effected with a Belgian breed, in which the males alone are
- streaked with black. In a similar manner, if any variation appeared in a
- female pigeon, which was from the first sexually limited in its development to
- the females, it would be easy to make a breed with the females alone thus
- characterized; but if the variation was not thus originally limited the
- process would be extremely difficult, perhaps impossible. ^469
-
- [Footnote 469: Since the publication of the first edition of this work, it has
- been highly satisfactory to me to find the following remarks (the "Field,"
- Sept., 1872) from so experienced a breeder as Mr. Tegetmeier. After describing
- some curious cases in pigeons, of the transmission of color by one sex alone,
- and the formation of a sub-breed with this character, he says: "It is a
- singular circumstance that Mr. Darwin should have suggested the possibility of
- modifying the sexual colors of birds by a course of artificial selection.
- When he did so, he was in ignorance of these facts that I have related; but it
- is remarkable how very closely he suggested the right method of procedure."]
-
- On the Relation Between the Period of Development of a Character and Its
- Transmission to One Sex or to Both Sexes. - Why certain characters should be
- inherited by both sexes and other characters by one sex alone, namely, by that
- sex in which the character first appeared, is in most cases quite unknown. We
- cannot even conjecture why with certain sub-breeds of the pigeon black striae,
- though transmitted through the female, should be developed in the male alone,
- while every other character is equally transferred to both sexes. Why, again,
- with cats, the tortoise-shell color should, with rare exceptions, be developed
- in the female alone. The very same character, such as deficient or
- supernumerary digits, color-blindness, etc., may with mankind be inherited by
- the males alone of one family, and in another family by the females alone,
- though in both cases transmitted through the opposite as well as through the
- same sex. ^470 Although we are thus ignorant, the two following rules seem
- often to hold good - that variations which first appear in either sex at a
- late period of life tend to be developed in the same sex alone; while
- variations which first appear early in life in either sex tend to be developed
- in both sexes. I am, however, far from supposing that this is the sole
- determining cause. As I have not elsewhere discussed this subject, and as it
- has an important bearing on sexual selection, I must here enter into lengthy
- and somewhat intricate details.
-
- [Footnote 470: References are given in my "Variation of Animals under
- Domestication," vol. ii, p. 72.]
-
- It is in itself probable that any character appearing at an early age
- would tend to be inherited equally by both sexes, for the sexes do not differ
- much in constitution before the power of reproduction is gained. On the other
- hand, after this power has been gained and the sexes have come to differ in
- constitution, the gemmules (if I may again use the language of pangenesis)
- which are cast off from each varying part in the one sex would be much more
- likely to possess the proper affinities for uniting with the tissues of the
- same sex and thus becoming developed than with those of the opposite sex.
-
- I was first led to infer that a relation of this kind exists from the
- fact that whenever and in whatever manner the adult male differs from the
- adult female, he differs in the same manner from the young of both sexes. The
- generality of this fact is quite remarkable; it holds good with almost all
- mammals, birds, amphibians and fishes; also with many crustaceans, spiders and
- some few insects, such as certain orthoptera and libellulae. In all these
- cases the variations, through the accumulation of which the male acquired his
- proper masculine characters, must have occurred at a somewhat late period of
- life; otherwise the young males would have been similarly characterized; and
- conformably with our rule, the variations are transmitted to and developed in
- the adult males alone. When, on the other hand, the adult male closely
- resembles the young of both sexes (these, with rare exceptions, being alike),
- he generally resembles the adult female; and in most of these cases the
- variations through which the young and old acquired their present characters,
- probably occurred, according to our rule, during youth. But there is here
- room for doubt, for characters are sometimes transferred to the offspring at
- an earlier age than that at which they first appeared in the parents, so that
- the parents may have varied when adult and have transferred their characters
- to their offspring while young. There are, moreover, many animals in which
- the two sexes closely resemble each other, and yet both differ from their
- young; and here the characters of the adults must have been acquired late in
- life; nevertheless, these characters, in apparent contradiction to our rule,
- are transferred to both sexes. We must not, however, overlook the possibility
- or even probability of successive variations of the same nature occurring,
- under exposure to similar conditions, simultaneously in both sexes at a rather
- late period of life; and in this case the variations would be transferred to
- the offspring of both sexes at a corresponding late age; and there would then
- be no real contradiction to the rule that variations occurring late in life
- are transferred exclusively to the sex in which they first appeared. This
- latter rule seems to hold true more generally than the second one, namely,
- that variations which occur in either sex early in life tend to be transferred
- to both sexes. As it was obviously impossible even to estimate in how large a
- number of cases throughout the animal kingdom these two propositions held
- good, it occurred to me to investigate some striking or crucial instances and
- to rely on the result.
-
-